Medindia
Personalized health information & news! Register
Medindia » Research News

Group Learning

Suparna Rajaram, a psychology professor tests how people learn and remember in groups.

by Sheela Philomena on April 30, 2011 at 11:33 AM

Suparna Rajaram, a psychology professor tests how people learn and remember in groups. Some studies have compared the recall of items on lists by "collaborative groups," or those who study together, and "nominal groups," in which individuals work alone and the results are collated.


Rajaram, at Stony Brook University, began to specialize in "collaborative memory".

Some findings in the field of collaborative memory research have been counter intuitive. For one, collaboration can hurt memory.

The collaborative groups remembered more items than any single person would have done alone. But they also remembered fewer than the nominal groups did by totalling the efforts of its solitary workers.

In other words, the collaborators' whole was less than the sum of its parts.

This so-called "collaborative inhibition" affects recall for all sorts of things, from word pairs to emotionally laden events; it affects strangers or spouses, children or adults. It is, in scientific lingo, "robust".

What explains this? One dynamic is "retrieval disruption": Each person remembers in his or her own way, and compelled to listen to others, can't use those strategies effectively.

Sometimes that effect fades. Sometimes it squashes the memories for good, causing "post collaborative forgetting". Then there's "social contagion" of errors, wherein a group member can implant erroneous recollections in another's memory.

On the other hand, collaborative learning helps, which is why people hold it in high esteem.

Individuals recall different information or events; after time, they can get together, contribute their bits, and re-educate each other's memories and expand the group's recall, mitigating the costs of collaboration.

People can also correct each other's erroneous memories, a process Rajaram and her colleagues call "error pruning". Or they can "cross-cue"-bring up recollections that jog memories others have forgotten.

Rajaram's work involves small groups in the controlled laboratory environment. Yet, like others in her field, she believes it can inform the understanding of the wider "networks in which social memory phenomena are occurring"-classrooms, institutions, communities, subcultures, or nations.

"If a small group can reshape memories, we see how individuals come to hold certain viewpoints or perspectives," she said.

"That can serve as a model for how collective identities and histories are shaped," she stated.

The findings have been published in Current Directions in Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

Source: ANI

View Non AMP Site | Back to top ↑